Photo: Obinna Anyadike/IRIN. Too much, too young: A therapeutic feeding centre in northern Nigeria
Source: IRIN
NEW YORK, 30 May 2014 (IRIN) - Scientific advances in human biology may
soon have a profound bearing on the policies that governments and
organizations adopt towards young mothers, caregivers and babies in poor
and stressed communities.
There is an emerging body of scientific evidence to show that the
environment in which a very young baby develops is pivotal in shaping
its brain in ways that can significantly influence its chances in later
life.
“Toxic stress”, depicting a relentless cycle of stress inflicted on a
child whose parents may be battling to survive, unable to nurture it
properly; or where there may be violence, neglect and/or poor nutrition,
can result in negative patterning on the baby’s brain that can inhibit
intellectual and emotional growth and hamper his or her chances of
success. This negative imprint, just like a positive imprint, gets
handed down from one generation to the next.
University of Cape Town-affiliated neurobiologist, Barak Morgan, says:
“We have known for a long time that early childhood is very important,
but now science is telling us exactly how important it is.” Whereas
educational policies tend to stress the importance of the early school
years, the new science suggests that the birth to three-year time frame
could be the most cost-effective and critical period for intervention in
a child’s life.
In a paper
entitled, “Biological embedding of early childhood adversity: Toxic
stress and the vicious cycle of poverty in South Africa”, Morgan
explains how signals from the environment are known to add permanent
“epigenetic marks” onto DNA during sensitive periods of early brain
development – both before and after birth. Then a period of resistance
settles in, where it becomes very difficult to change these pathways. It
is not known exactly when all of these brief, sensitive windows occur,
but the first two to three years in a child’s life are critical for him
or her to acquire emotional self-regulation skills that make the
difference between failure and success later in life.
Spare the rod
Groundbreaking research
by Canadian Michael Meany several years ago on lab rats showed that the
amount of licking and grooming the baby rats received in their first
days of life determined their responses to stress.
Those that received minimal licking and grooming were primed to survive
their environment with “flight or fight” responses” by having more
epigenetic marks on the brain’s major stress gene. Pups that were licked
and groomed adequately, regardless of whether by their biological
mother or a surrogate mother, had less of these marks, which made them
resilient to stress and primed to thrive. Later studies on humans, also
in Canada, showed that the brains of suicide victims had more epigenetic
marks, similar to those of the poorly nurtured rat pups.
“The shape and impact of these pathways are sculpted in a once-off way
during early development when environmental influences, as mediated by
parental care, are most deeply embedded in offspring biology,” Morgan
writes. Crucially, the epigenetic marks hamper the development of more
sophisticated, flexible “top down” brain functioning that is associated
with strong self-regulation and the ability to thrive, as distinguished
from more reflex, survival-oriented, “bottom up” brain functioning.
Morgan
writes that children with poor self-regulation are shown to mature into
adults with “significantly higher rates of substance dependence,
criminality, financial problems and single parenthood, and significantly
lower income, financial planning skills, socioeconomic status and
physical health.”
He adds: “Only very recently has interdisciplinary neuroscience begun to
reveal and characterize the stress of chronic poverty as a major
environmental toxin that becomes embedded in the biological fabric of
bodies and minds in ways that cripple healthy development.”
Nature and nurture
Things have moved very fast since these fairly recent research findings
and an entire body of literature is now emerging around “how the
environment gets embedded in biology,” he says. The new findings render
the “nature” versus “nurture” debate obsolete. Morgan says it is now
clear that genes and the environment form an inseparable whole. “Genes
can do nothing alone, something in the environment must tell DNA when
and what to do. Nature versus Nurture turns out to be Nature and Nurture,” he writes.
Furthermore, this embedding of the environment in the genes “gains
intergenerational momentum”, he says. The lab rat research shows that
rat pups that were not licked and groomed properly, matured into adults
that did not nurture their own pups properly either. This helps to
explain how the cycle of poverty and deprivation can be reinforced from
one generation to the next.
While there is always room for new science, those in the field are not
contesting the latest findings. Some argue that there is enough evidence
at hand to nudge governments into implementing new policies that aim to
reduce the impact of violence and poverty on babies and toddlers.
Leading the pack is Harvard University’s Center on the Developing Child
director, Jack Shonkoff, an expert in child health and development.
After a recent UNICEF panel discussion
by scientists on the subject, Shonkoff said in a televised interview:
“The quality of the foundation we build in the first couple of years
doesn't completely determine everything that is going to come later, but
it nevertheless sets you up for a lifetime of good prospects for
healthy development, or it puts you in a deep hole that says the risks
are much greater that you will have problems across the board.”
In the interview Shonkoff says that the new science is leading to a much
deeper understanding of why poor nutrition is so problematic in early
childhood. “It’s not just because kids are not growing well but it’s
because their brain development is affected by it.” He adds that
interventions that merely offer stimulation for children in poor
communities are inadequate. “It’s a matter of figuring out how to
protect their developing brain from the stress – toxic stress –
associated with chronic exposure to violence, really deep poverty and
the day-to-day stress of just barely getting by.”
Lawrence Aber, a psychologist from New York University’s Steinhardt
School of Culture, Education and Human Development, is an expert on the
impact of poverty and violence on early child development. “Before,
policymakers didn’t think very young children would be affected so badly
because they didn’t talk or didn’t seem to be aware,” he says. “Now
however, science has shown just the opposite. In fact they are highly
sensitive to their environment – more so than at any other time in their
life cycle.” Aber is involved in research on how HIV/AIDs and poverty
impacts on children in South Africa.
Policy implications
The new research has profound and practical implications for policy, he
believes. “Our knowledge about what is happening inside a child’s brain
must direct our attention outside – to the environment – and to reducing
poverty and violence.” Later exposure in life to these negatives can
also impact a child’s development but early exposure seems to be the
most scarring. “The earlier the twig is bent the more likely it is to
grow in the direction you bend it. The earliest years are the most
vulnerable years,” he adds.
But what is the prognosis for the countless babies who have already been
exposed to toxic stress? The science seems to suggest that early damage
is irreversible. This is not so, say the scientists, however. “It is
not just a story of doom and gloom, or a case of once bent always
broken,” says Aber. “One must remember that human beings – including
very young and vulnerable ones – have enormous capacity for resilience,
or what developmental scientists call plasticity. Children can bounce
back. We can help them recover from the toxic stress they experience in
infancy and toddlerhood.”
Morgan also sites research on Romanian babies in institutions that shows
the importance of timing to counteract the effects of stress. The
cognitive and social outcomes for severely emotionally deprived babies
adopted before 20 months of age were as good as those of their Canadian
and British peers who had not been deprived. However, for those adopted
after 20 months of age, the prognosis was less rosy. There is also the
“dandelions” and “orchids” hypothesis, which suggests that some children
(dandelions) are less genetically predisposed to being influenced –
positively or negatively - by their environment than others who are more
sensitive (orchids).
While there is growing awareness of the need for investment in children
long before they enter school, Aber believes that not everyone is ready
to act. “The logic is irrefutable but we haven’t made the policy
investments yet,” he says, adding however that governments in developing
countries “are starting to realize that if they don’t affect change in
the first years of a child’s life there will be a glass ceiling on what
can be achieved in the education system and ultimately, on national
productivity.”
For children living in poverty in South Africa, where domestic violence
rates are high, toxic stress “is a newly recognized pandemic that must
be addressed,” Morgan argues.
Invest in children
But what kind of interventions can be made? Aber sites the benefits of
the “conditional cash transfer” system on poverty alleviation efforts in
Latin America and increasingly, Africa and South Asia.
“These cash payments to very poor families are conditional on the
family’s investment in the child’s development,” he says. These schemes
encourage mothers to attend antenatal classes, get their babies
immunized and their children to attend school, for example. They can be
adopted to help minimize the effects of poverty and violence too.
Visits from health workers to new mothers have been shown to improve parental practices in Khayelitsha,
South Africa, for example. Programs that help parents understand the
dangers of exposing their young children to toxic stress, and help them
find ways to shield them from it, can help too.
But what is called for, says Aber, is far more than small-scale
programmatic intervention. “The entire system needs to address this on a
broad population level.”